In 1907, Chelpanov accepted the philosophy chair in Moscow university on the condition that the university would fund a laboratory for experimental psychology. Chelpanov had become interested in psychology attending lectures by A. N. Giliarov (1855−1938), the philosophy professor at the St. Vladimir university in Kiev. He had then travelled to Leipzig to work with Wundt and, while teaching in Kiev, started a course in psychology with laboratory work. When Moscow University appointed him, he asked for a suitable space for a laboratory and for a reading room. All the same, at first the laboratory had no assistant positions, and Chelpanov’s first students, Konstantin Nikolaevich Kornilov (1879−1957) and N. A. Rybnikov (1880−1961) were his unpaid assistants. Together with a laboratory worker, Gubarev, they got the apparatus to work using Wundt’s and Titchener’s handbooks; at the end they were able to publish their own laboratory guide for experimental psychology.
Chelpanov announced a three-year course in psychology based on laboratory work and a well-structured teaching seminar. The topic for the first year was the mind-body problem, for the second, psychology’s subject matter, and for the third there was a range of topics from the psychology of attention and thinking to personality and functionalism. There was also a preparatory course, or "pre-seminar," at the end of which students took examinations in psychology and in the German language and had to write an essay; only after this could students enroll in experimental psychology proper. Chelpanov’s course was highly successful and it gave him ambitions to expand the laboratory. This became possible when a wealthy Muscovite, S. I. Shchukin, donated a large sum of money for the construction of a psychological institute for the university, in a building in which it still exists. Chelpanov traveled in Europe and the U.S.A. to see existing institutes; the result was a luxurious four-storey building with well-equipped laboratories, opening formally on 23 March 1914 (Botsmanova, Guseva, Ravich-Shcherbo 1994).
To appreciate the role and history of this institute, which is in a way a microcosm of larger events, it is necessary to broaden the historical outlook. The list of practitioners who claimed some expertise in psychology was, by 1914, much larger that the list of those who were primarily philosophers. There was considerable interest in applied fields in Russia, connected in many cases with the socialist movement. In 1903, "in the interests of protecting the working class from physical and moral degeneration, as well as developing the workers' ability to fight for themselves," the first program of the Russian Socialist Democratic Workers Party demanded an eight-hour working day (quoted in Kostiuk, Mikulinskii, and Yaroshevskii 1980, p. 41). In the same year, Sechenov conducted what turned out to be his last laboratory research, on fatigue. The elderly man experimented on himself lifting a weight till he felt exhausted. He and other intellectuals were acutely aware of the problems created by the repressive tsarist regime, and they looked to science for answers. In this respect, the aspirations of psychologists coincided with those of the revolutionaries, and this proved to be to the advantage of psychology’s institutionalization.
The early Bolsheviks — at least, the educated members of the party — were true believers in science and, after the disastrous years of the European war, the Revolution, and the Civil War, gave it considerable attention. This interest, however, was double-edged. Bolshevik science policy favored the large scale, and in time it channeled massive resources into industry and research. It founded, and funded from the state budget, new research institutes and laboratories, some of them — like Pavlov’s — huge, and it gave existing facilities status as national, state institutions. As a result, however, science became the monopoly of a highly centralized state system, in which considerable numbers of warring bureaucracies had an interest, and scholars became state employees with no real autonomy. In the conditions of centralized direction of intellectual and academic thought, as well as administration, which the state imposed on all sectors, including psychology, from the late 1920s, this had severe consequences. At times, in circumstances where there was intense competition for state funds and influence in science, this led to calamitous, sometimes vicious, practices. After late 1921, when Lenin’s orders led to the expulsion of a large number of intellectuals from the country, with members of the Moscow and Petrograd psychological societies among them, for resistance to the materialist struggle, it gradually became clear that the state would not allow scientists independently to judge the basis of objective knowledge. (St. Petersburg was renamed Petrograd during the war against Germany and Austria.)
The history of Chelpanov’s institute exemplifies the early changes. Like other areas of Moscow university, the institute survived 1917, even the anxious and cold winter (the building was not heated). It managed to publish three issues of the journal, Psikhologicheskoe obozrenie (Psychological review), which had been started on the eve of the Revolution. Then, in 1918 and 1919, the new Soviet ministry of education, Narkompros (People's Commissariat of Enlightenment) recognized the institute’s existence by financing the laboratory. In 1920, Chelpanov completed his blueprint for a "universal psychological apparatus," which combined several traditional laboratory devices and would cost less then a set of separate instruments, and he asked the government for funding. The same year, he opened a new section on applied and work psychology.
Even before the Revolution, Russian academics had nurtured a project for a network of research institutes, either within the universities or separate. After 1917, some of them, those willing to collaborate with the new authorities, again voiced this plan. The Bolsheviks in turn gave their support, partly because it satisfied their taste for the grandiose, partly because it fitted in with their scientism. The leading Party philosopher, Nikolai Bukharin, set the tone with a much to be quoted reference to the theory of conditional reflexes as "a weapon from the iron arsenal of materialism" (quoted in Joravsky 1989, p. 226). The political situation encouraged a conception of research institutes as "big science" on the mass-production or industrial scale thought to be integral to communism.
In the background was the utopian image of "the New Man," with its long history, the vision of refashioning human nature for the new times, shaping the future. "The New Man" embodied the hope that objective, materialist knowledge and a just social order would make possible a new human nature (Bauer, 1952). Aleksei Kapitonovich Gastev (1882−1941), a poet and a revolutionary, who expressed the view "that the human being is nothing but a perfect machine … and that the technical progress of this machine is unlimited" (quoted in Kozulin 1984, p. 16), became director of the Central Institute of Work, founded to rationalize labor processes. If Gastev’s ideas were utopian, the institute was nevertheless home to both Isaak Naftul’evich Shpil’rein (1891−1937) and Nikolai Bernshtein. The former developed testing, psychotechnics, used for occupational placement; and this field grew to such an extent that the International Psychotechnical Congress came to Russia in 1931. Bernshtein began to develop a non-mechanistic approach to human motor activity, a major intellectual challenge to Pavlovian theory. Along with Gastev, Aron Borisovich Zalkind (1888−1936), who held positions in the new communist academic institutions founded to raise a generation of Marxist-educated cadres, strongly supported the idea of the plasticity of human nature. He put forward his ideas, drawing like so many others on a reflex model, in the fields of education and testing. Zalkind argued that the business of psychology is to construct a new human personality along with the new society. In practice, this collapsed into recommendations for adjusting individuals to what was demanded of them.
Academic changes began at the end of 1921, when the Bolsheviks, overcoming resistance, succeeded in reforming Moscow university. The new government started to attack traditional notions of academic freedom by replacing old professors with "red" ones and by changing the student body. It instructed the universities to enroll everybody, regardless of previous education. The universities had to establish so-called workers' departments (rabfak), because the majority of new students were not able to follow the lectures. The Party viewed this sector as a tool for bringing universities under its control. From the first year of its existence, 1920, the rabfak of Moscow university enrolled more students than all the other departments together. This large-scale affirmative action brought to the universities an army of students eager to make a career, often at the expense of studying. Every university, like all other institutions, had a Party cell, with members recruited mainly from the workers' departments. The reform of university administration then took the form of establishing "people's councils," as opposed to academic ones, where students from the working class sat side-by-side with professors. At the same time, the government founded new institutions, such as the Communist Academy, specifically to train people "from below" in Bolshevik principles, and these new "red" scientists and administrators were antagonistic to the old scholarly élite. In psychology, the opposition to élite "idealism" came out into the open at the first post-war Russian psychoneurological congress, held in Moscow in 1923, and especially in the public reporting of the event which side-lined Chelpanov’s work in favor of non-mentalist contributions.
In order to facilitate "Marxist leadership," the Bolsheviks merged all the departments where the humanities were traditionally taught into one department of the social sciences, and they united all the institutes, where post-graduates prepared dissertations, into an administratively centralized Association. One of the Association’s first decisions was to legislate that only Marxist professors could teach a number of disciplines, including sociology, comparative ethnology, philosophy, and modern history. It also made an exam in Marxist philosophy mandatory for all doctoral candidates. It could sign-up and sack teachers and students on ideological grounds, and it controlled each institute’s funds. In November 1921, the psychological institute became part of the Association, and the university permanently lost administrative control of the institute.
Nevertheless, alongside these controls, and in spite of a shortage of everything, the post-revolutionary period was a time of significant quantitative growth. Before 1917, the Moscow institute had only one salaried position, the director’s; by 1922, there were about thirty posts. Besides the director, there were three "full members," including Chelpanov’s eldest assistant, Kornilov. The "first-category researchers" included four persons from the younger generation — B. N. Severnyi, V. M. Ekzempliarskii, P. P. Sokolov, and N. N. Ladygina-Kots; the latter two had followed Chelpanov’s courses and started working at the laboratory before the Revolution. (Ladygina-Kots later became known for her research on primates.) Yet nearly half of positions remained vacant, so Chelpanov planned to open special courses in psychology for prospective researchers.
By the summer of 1922, however, Chelpanov’s own position as director was endangered: an "old" university professor, a philosopher known for his critique of materialism, he stood out against the new "red" background. The main attack came from within the institute, from Kornilov, who, before he came to Moscow, was a teacher in a remote Siberian town. Kornilov had for some time worked on "reactology," his own version of experimental psychology, involving the "objective" registration of reactions as opposed to introspection or self-observation. He had kept it half-secret from Chelpanov, a loyal Wundtian, and he pursued his enthusiasm exclusively at home seminars. Kornilov found the post-revolutionary climate more sympathetic to his research and published his results in a book, Reaktologiia (Reactology, 1922).
Kornilov claimed that his reactology was ideologically correct, while Chelpanov’s philosophy was idealist and hostile to Marxism. In fact, reactology came into existence at a time when Kornilov, according to his own words, "had a poor knowledge of Marxist philosophy" (quoted in Sirotkina, 2006, p. 261). But Kornilov, the former schoolteacher, was preferable as the director of a Soviet institution than the élite professor. The change of directorship took place as an administrative reform. In December 1922, the Association of research institutes of Moscow university closed the psychological institute and made its staff redundant. The same resolution announced a psychological section in the newly founded institute of scientific philosophy, with Kornilov as the head.
After his dismissal, Chelpanov wrote to L. D. Trotsky, who had some knowledge and interest in psychology. Deeply resentful of losing his life’s business, the old professor ended up by adopting Marxist rhetoric in fighting his opponents; this was a foretaste of much future conflict. But Trotsky, already out of favor with the Party, either could not or did not want to help. Kornilov, for his part, easily dismissed his old teacher’s complaints: if he, Kornilov, was a bad Marxist, Chelpanov was no Marxist at all. Chelpanov was sheltered by one of the few students who remained loyal, Shpet, who, as the president of the newly founded Russian academy of the art sciences, a state-funded institution for art studies, appointed Chelpanov to head the academy’s psychological section.
When it changed hands, the institute lost Moscow university funds and did not receive any from the institute of scientific philosophy, whose budget had been drafted before it acquired the psychology section. The section nevertheless, formally, had seven research posts, and Kornilov applied to increase them to twenty. To fill vacant positions, he invited the Marxist sociologist, M. A. Reisner, Chelpanov’s former student turned "objective" psychologist, P. P. Blonskii (1884−1941), and an animal psychologist from Odessa, V. K. Borovskii. "Junior researchers" included Luria, from Kazan', who became the academic secretary of the institute, the specialists in psychotechnics, S. G. Gellershtein (1896−1967) and Isaak Shpil’rein, and the psychoanalyst Sabina Shpil’rein (Spilrein, 1885−1942, the sister of the latter).
Kornilov also applied to the Association of research institutes to restore the psychological institute, and in the summer of 1924 it became the Moscow State Institute of Experimental Psychology. It was "reborn," as Luria, remembered, under the sign of reactology (quoted in Levitin 1991, pp. 130−131; also Luria 1979). This meant that all research referred to "reaction": Kornilov studied "reactions of maximal inhibition;" Vygotsky examined "dominant reactions;" Bernshtein the impact of "reaction on the shape of the movement;" and so on. The term had different meanings, however, and, in spite of the rhetoric of Marxist materialism, Kornilov himself kept to the classical notion of reaction in his experiments. Psychotechnicians resorted less often to the language of "reaction;" because their work had practical importance, it was easier for them to justify it to the authorities.
Under the guise of new terminology, Kornilov’s staff pursued their own research interests. For instance, Luria’s reference to "affective reactions" was in fact a euphemism for a topic related to psychoanalysis. Working at the institute, he asked experimental subjects to respond to the stimulus word by an association while at the same time pressing the button of a dynamoscope devised by Kornilov. By "the joint motor method," he and Leont’ev, who had come to the institute in 1923, studied "affective complexes" both in students during exams and in suspects under criminal investigation, leading to a story, reported by Kornilov, about Luria inventing a lie detector. Working with Kornilov, Vygotsky used the term "reaction" in his 1925 dissertation and in an article, "Problema dominantnykh reaktsii" (The problem of the dominant reactions, 1926). (Yet the choice of words was not so important to him and he used "reaction" and "reflex" as synonyms.)
To compete with Chelpanov’s Vvedenie v eksperimental’nuiu psikhologiiu (Introduction to experimental psychology, 1915), Kornilov published a new Praktikum po eksperimental’noi psikhologii (Practical guide to experimental psychology, 1927), written from the viewpoint of reactology. The junior staff — Bernshtein, Gellershtein, Luria, and Vygotsky — wrote most of the chapters, although they shared doubts about reactology’s heuristic value. Kornilov’s great ambition was to resolve the mind-body problem by means of experimental research. Vygotsky was skeptical; and Gellershtein also described the difficulties which arise in trying to analyze the relationships between mental and physical processes.
In 1928, Bernshtein, Leont’ev, and Vygotsky left the institute, and Luria was replaced as academic secretary by a Marxist philosopher, Yu. V. Frankfurt, who was so influential that he nearly became academic secretary of the entire Russian Association of research institutes in the social sciences (the descendant of the Moscow university Association). At the same time, Kornilov, Zalkind, and Blonskii started feeling the consequences of the project that they had initiated. The genie of "Marxist psychology," once let out of the bottle, gradually accumulated power. Psychologists spent time on endless ideological discussions. In 1930, a specially organized meeting, "the reactological discussion," accused Kornilov of not being loyal to Marxism. His main critics were postgraduate students from the institute, well schooled in the official philosophy (Umrikhin 1991). In 1931, Zalkind and then V. N. Kolbanovskii, whose only virtue was that he was an orthodox Marxist, replaced Kornilov. Yet, in the 1930s, the number of staff at the institute increased to about one hundred.
This expansion was dependent on claims made for psychology as practice. The socialist state, beginning in the twenties, created numerous positions that psychologists could occupy in the army, education, healthcare, social welfare, and so on, and the expanding number of psychology students made it easy to fill these places. At the same time, universities faced rivalry as sites for research from newly-founded academic institutes, industry, educational institutions, and other places for applied research. It was symptomatic that Chelpanov’s other elder student (besides Kornilov), Rybnikov, having had enough of the contested area of experimentation, moved into the relatively quiet area of the study of children.
In the climate of social reconstruction in the 1920s, it was easy to persuade institutions to establish positions for psychologists. Former experimental psychologists (including Chelpanov himself) worked in art studies, medicine, education, "the scientific organization of labor," and sport. The army was an important employer, and it established psychological laboratories within the ministry of defense and other military bodies. The staff of the psychological institute set up sections in social, child, and animal psychology, and in psychotechnics and psychopathology. Its staff also worked on testing soldiers with what were called "the American methods," for the purpose of successful communication compiled the "dictionary of the Red Army soldier," and contributed to the training of army officers and Communist Party instructors. The science of psychology was in demand in a society making a conscious effort to modernize itself and to proclaim the creation of "the New Man." Psychologists successfully claimed usefulness in the Soviet Union, and the state rewarded them with the field’s institutionalization.
In the Stalinist state, however, this support had another side- almost complete dependency — and as a result psychology, as well as other sciences, suffered from the repressive policy of the state. With the beginning of the "Great Break" and Stalin’s "cultural revolution", control over science included severe censorship and regular attacks on scientists modeled on political campaigns. One of the first campaigns organized in psychology was so-called "reactological discussion" in 1931 that finished Kornilov’s reactology and led to his dismissal from the Moscow institute. Sometimes, as a result, entire disciplines were eliminated, as was the case with psychotechnics and pedology. To illustrate the mechanics of these campaigns, we will examine the fate of pedology, the science of the child, in the Soviet Union.